Feelings of Trust, Distrust and Risky Decision-Making in Political Office. An Experimental Study With National Politicians in Three Democracies

Published date01 June 2023
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1177/00104140221139376
AuthorJames Weinberg
Date01 June 2023
Subject MatterArticles
Article
Comparative Political Studies
2023, Vol. 56(7) 935967
© The Author(s) 2022
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/00104140221139376
journals.sagepub.com/home/cps
Feelings of Trust,
Distrust and Risky
Decision-Making in
Political Off‌ice. An
Experimental Study With
National Politicians in
Three Democracies
James Weinberg
1
Abstract
Tackling an important gap in the literature on political trust, this article fo-
cuses on politicians and the relevance of their other-to-self trust judgements
for decision-making in public off‌ice. A unique quantitative dataset gathered
from national politicians in the UK, Canada and South Africa is used to (1)
examine descriptive levels of felt trust and distrust among politicians and (2)
evaluate the impact of these feelings on politiciansrisky decision-making. To
achieve outcome (2), this article presents the results of three survey ex-
periments in which politicians were asked to make decisions in scenarios
where both the presentation and the nature of risk varied. The results indicate
that MPsperceptions of public trust and distrust do matter for risky decision-
making, and that these variables moderate a ref‌lection effect whereby MPs are
otherwise more risk-averse in the face of gains and risk-taking in the face of
losses.
1
University of Sheff‌ield, Sheff‌ield, UK
Corresponding Author:
James Weinberg, The Department of Politics and International Relations, University of Sheff‌ield,
Elmf‌ield Building, Sheff‌ield S10 2TN, UK.
Email: james.weinberg@sheff‌ield.ac.uk
Keywords
risk, trust, distrust, decision-making, ref‌lection effect, politicians, heuristic
Introduction
Despite the existence of an expansive literature attesting to the importance of
political trust for public opinion, participation and policy compliance (for a
detailed review, see Citrin & Stoker,2018 ), it is astonishing that so little of it, if
any, has focused upon how the existence of a low-trust, high-blame envi-
ronment affects politiciansdecision-making and thus the quality of public
governance. Given that political trust is routinely understood as a relational
concept involving both citizens (the trustors) and politicians (the trustees),
it is somewhat remarkable that there remains no knowledge of if or how the
latter interpret and act upon political trust. These questions represent a missing
piece in the trust puzzle as studied in political science and cognate disciplines.
Tackling this gap head on, I take a novel dialogic approach to vertical trust-
based relationships that gives equal weight to politiciansfeelings of being
trusted and distrusted within the principal-agent relationship that characterises
representative democracies.
1
In this article, I focus on one particularly sig-
nif‌icant dimension of this research agenda: when, why and how these feelings
might inf‌luence politiciansrisky decision-making. Risk is, of course, endemic
to political life. By virtue of their off‌ice, politicians must constantly make
decisions with very real and consequential personal and public risks attached.
In turn, risks per se are interwoven with the psychological fabric of trust and
distrust, which together represent an evolutionary response to ontological
vulnerability and uncertainty (see Cai et al., 2020;Mackenzie, 2020).
However, research on the trust-risk relationship has thus far overlooked an
important question: do politiciansperceptions of public trust or distrust in
themselves impact their propensity to engage in risk-seeking or risk-averse
behaviours? To theorise this link more precisely, I draw on the trust-as-
heuristic tradition (see Hetherington, 2005;Rudolph, 2017), which takes
political trust as a decision rule that assists citizens as they decide whether or
not to support government action. Specif‌ically, I f‌lip this logic 180° to suggest
that f‌luid and variegated perceptions of public trust and distrust act as a
heuristic for politicians who not only occupy a job laden with risk but exist in
an overwhelmingly complex informational environment (for a discussion of
elitesuse of heuristics, see Vis, 2019).
In this article, I propose and test this thesis for the f‌irst time using original
survey measures and experiments administered to more than 100 nationally
elected politicians drawn from three major democracies (the UK, Canada and
South Africa). I examine the relationship between politiciansperceptions of
public [dis]trust and their decisions in scenarios where the nature and
936 Comparative Political Studies 56(7)
presentation of risk differs. I f‌ind preliminary evidence that MPsperceptions
of public trust and distrust do matter for risky decision-making, and that these
variables moderate a ref‌lection effect whereby MPs are otherwise more risk-
averse in the face of gains and risk-taking in the face of losses. The con-
tribution of this article is three-fold. Theoretically, I advance existing work on
trust and governance by demonstrating the importance of politiciansother-to-
self perceptions of trust-related concepts. Methodologically, I add to a small
but important research base that has used experimental methods with political
elites (for a review, see Kertzer & Renshon, 2022) and I test a new battery of
felttrust and distrust with politicians in comparative contexts. Empirically, I
use these methods to show that MPs are not only susceptible to well-
researched biases in some risk-laden scenarios, but that these cognitive
shortcuts can interact with [seemingly inaccurate] appraisals of public opinion
to help MPs reach behavioural decisions in the presence of uncertainty.
What Does it Mean to be Trusted and Why Does it Matter?
As an essentially contested concept, there continues to be f‌ierce debate about
whether political trust is a unidimensional, holistically conceived attitude (e.g.
Hetherington & Rudolph, 2008;Hooghe, 2011) or whether it is, as David
Chan (2019, p. 3) argues, decidedly multi-level, multi-dimensional and mal-
leable. The formerinterpretation has tended to dominatea relatively traditional
and longstandingapproach to the measurement oftrust in mass surveys such as
the American National Election Study (ANES), the General Social Survey
(GSS) and Gallup,whilst the latter has informeda more nuanced, target-specif‌ic
study of trust and distrust across politics as well as the organisational and
psychological sciences (Devine et al., 2020;Hamm et al., 2019). Whilst it is
neither necessary norpossible to explore this theoretical debate in detailin this
article, it is worthclarifying my position by way of foregrounding later remarks.
I distinguish here between trust as an action (decisions or behaviours) and
trust as a series of internalised psychological processes that inform those
actions. This approach relies on a number of core assumptions: trust in all
forms is relational, it depends upon reciprocal multi-faceted judgements about
trustor and trustee, and it is interrelated to but distinguishable from distrust. On
the f‌irst point, scholars such as Peter Nannestad (2008, p. 417) and Russell
Hardin (2002) have advanced a sound understanding of trust as a three-way
relationshipwhere A trusts some specif‌ic B with respect to some specif‌icx.
The important takeaway here is that trust must be conceived as something that
is target-specif‌ic and granted contingently within a context-specif‌ic domain of
action. On the second point, the contingencies implicit in this relational model
are resolved by trust judgements about the trustworthiness of another. To be
precise, people make cognitive, affective and behavioural-intentional
judgements about potential trustees (e.g., about their ability, benevolence
Weinberg 937

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT