Exercise to Lose Money? Irrational Exercise Behavior from the Chinese Warrants Market

AuthorWeiqiang Zhang,Zhisheng Li,Li Liao,Ning Zhu
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1002/fut.21608
Date01 May 2014
Published date01 May 2014
EXERCISE TO LOSE MONEY?
IRRATIONAL EXERCISE BEHAVIOR
FROM THE CHINESE WARRANTS MARKET
LI LIAO, ZHISHENG LI, WEIQIANG ZHANG and NING ZHU*
Using a marketlevel exercise data set and an individuallevel trading data set between
August 2006 and June 2009, this study examinesthe incidence of two types of irrational exercise
behavior in the Chinese warrants market. We nd that 121.64 million shares of warrants (0.64%
of all warrants) were either exercised with an immediateloss or failed to be exercised, resulting in
foregone riskfree prots. These irrational exercises caused warrant holders to lose over 717.79
million Yuan. Some of the irrational behavior can be attributed to entertainment seekingand
the Tþ1rule practiced in the Chinese security market, but the majority is attributed to
warrant holdersignorance and/or negligence of warrant mechanics. Our ndings provide
additional eld evidence of clearly irrational exercise behavior in a derivatives market. We also
nd that investor education, information and guidance provision can mitigate the incidence of
irrational exercise behavior signicantly. © 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Jrl Fut Mark
34:399419, 2014
1. INTRODUCTION
Evidence of behavioral mistakes in the nancial markets is increasing. Given that increasing
studies (Barber, Lee, Liu, & Odean, 2009; Barber, Odean, & Zhu, 2009; Hvidkjaer, 2008;
Kaniel,Saar, & Titman, 2008;Kumar & Lee, 2006) showthat such irrationalbehavioral patterns
by investors can have substantial impact on price formation and liquidity in the securities
markets, more needs to be known as to why investors make some seemingly simple mistakes.
One challenge in answering such a q uestion is that prior studies rely heavily on
specic assumptions about invest or utility (Campbell, 2006; Dhar & Zhu, 20 06; Grinblatt
& Keloharju, 2001; Heath, Huddar t, & Lang, 1999; Lee, Liu, & Zhu, 2008; Odean, 19 98),
Li Liao is a Professor at PBC School of Finance at Tsinghua University, Beijing, Peoples Republic of China.
Zhisheng Li is a Professor at the School of Finance at Zhongnan University of Economics and Law, Wuhan,
Peoples Republic of China. Weiqiang Zhang is a Postdoctoral Researcher in School of Economics and
Management at Tsinghua University, Beijing, Peoples Republic of China. Ning Zhu is a Professor at Shanghai
Advanced Institute of Finance (SAIF), Shanghai Jiaotong University, Shanghai, Peoples Republic of China,
and UC Davis, Davis, California. We are very grateful to the anonymous brokerage rm for making available
the data used in the study and answering questions related to the data. The authors beneted from comments
from Charles Cao, Neil Pearson, seminar participants at SAIF, an anonymous referee, and Bob Webb (the
editor). We acknowledge nancial support from the TsinghuaCiti Financial Research and Education
Program. Liao, Zhang, and Zhu acknowledge Project 71232003 supported by NSFC. Li acknowledges project
71271214 and 70801063 supported by NSFC. Zhu acknowledges Project 70972009 and 70932002 supported
by NSFC. All remaining errors are ours only.
*Correspondence author, Shanghai Jiaotong University, Shanghai, Peoples Republic of China, and UC Davis, Davis,
CA. Tel: þ86216293 2280, Fax: þ86216293 2310, email: nzhu@ucdavis.edu
Received October 2011; Accepted January 2013
The Journal of Futures Markets, Vol. 34, No. 5, 399419 (2014)
© 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Published online 19 March 2013 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com).
DOI: 10.1002/fut.21608
whereas derivatives markets p rovide a unique opportunity to study inv estor behavior
because the valuation of many der ivative securities can be calculated pre cisely without
invoking particular assumptions ab out expectation or investor utility function. Fol lowing
such logic, prior studies indee d document that investors fail to properly exercise American
options before expiration in the U.S. options market (Diz & Finuc ane, 1993; Overdahl,
1988; Pool, Stoll, & Whaley, 2008). Al though some studies point out that market fricti on
and institutional considerati ons are responsible for the irrational exerci se behavior
(Dawson, 1996; Pool et al., 2008), Po teshman and Serbin (2003) show that part of the
irrational exercise behavior can be a ttributed precisely to irrational behaviora l decisions.
Building upon the previous studies on early option exercise behavior, the current study
looks at a different aspect of optimal option exercise behavior in the context of the Chinese
warrants market, where investors face a relatively simple decision upon warrant expiration:
whether or not to exercise the warrants that they hold.
1
Because the strike price and the market price of the underlying stock are easy to identify,
it seems straightforward to decide whether to exercise a particular warrant. However, we
indeed nd that Chinese investors frequently fail to exercise valuable warrants upon
expiration and sometimes exercise outofthemoney warrants that cause them to lose
money. Among all 39 warrants, we nd irrational exercise behavior of some kind for 35
warrants (89.74% of the sample). In terms of the total number of expiring warrant shares,
121.64 million out of 19,002.12 million shares were identied as irrational exercise (0.64%).
Our calculations reveal that investors have lost over 700 million Yuan (over 100 million USD)
by failing to properly exercise the warrant contracts that they hold.
By investigating the incidenc e of irrational exercise behavior for differe nt types of
warrant contracts, we gain so me insights for reasons behind the app arent irrational
exercise behavior. First, we nd that the Tþ1del ivery mechanism in the Chinese stoc k
market is partly responsibl e for our ndings. Because investor s would have to hold the
shares that they receive from w arrant exercise for at least 1 day befor e they can liquidate
such stocks in the stock market for call warrants and pur chase the underlying shares at
least one trading day prior to exe rcise for put warrants, they face poten tial risks from the
next day price movement. Once we control for the 10% price movement limit in the next
day of trading (the maximum p otential risk one may face when decid ing whether to
exercise the warrant), the frac tion of irrational exercise decreases to 0.53 % of all warrant
shares (from 0.64% when not ac counting for the Tþ1trading rule), yet still remains
prevalent.
Secondly,we nd that part of the irrational exercise behavior can be attributedto investors
lack of knowledge about warrant contracts and their lack of attention when warrant contracts
come to expiration. Investors are far less likely to irrationally exercise their warrants at the
Shenzhen Stock Exchange (0.10% of all warrant shares), which requires warrant issuers to
publicly disclose the valueof warrants and the optimal exercise decision upon expiration,than
those at the Shanghai Stock Exchange (0.74% of all warrant shares), which does not impose
similar requirement for information disclosure related to warrant exercise.
The current study provides two primary contributions to the extant literature. First, we
provide some novel evidence of clearly irrational trading behavior when Chinese investors
trade derivatives in the emerging derivatives market.
The Chinese warrants market creates at least three unique opportunities for studying
irrational investor behavior. First, unlike other previously documented behavioral biases in
investor trading that require certain assumptions about investorsutility functions (Dhar &
1
In general, the stock exchange or brokerage rms do not automatically exercise inthemoney warrants on behalf of
investors.
400 Liao et al.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT