Evaluating Self-Control Theory Among the Deaf Community

AuthorTony R. Smith,Jason D. Scott,Judy L. Porter,LaVerne McQuiller Williams
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1177/0306624X211049186
Published date01 April 2023
Date01 April 2023
Subject MatterArticles
https://doi.org/10.1177/0306624X211049186
International Journal of
Offender Therapy and
Comparative Criminology
2023, Vol. 67(5) 524 –545
© The Author(s) 2021
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/0306624X211049186
journals.sagepub.com/home/ijo
Article
Evaluating Self-Control
Theory Among the Deaf
Community
Tony R. Smith1, Jason D. Scott1, Judy L. Porter1,
and LaVerne McQuiller Williams1
Abstract
This study evaluates the generality of self-control theory with a previously untested
cultural group rarely studied by criminologists, the Deaf community. Survey data
(n = 428) from participants attending a university that houses a college for the
Deaf and hard-of-hearing were compared with a sample of “hearing” students.
The findings support Gottfredson and Hirschi’s cultural invariance thesis as self-
control was consistently able to predict a wide range of rule-breaking behaviors
among the culturally distinct groups examined. However, several unexpected results
challenge the parental management thesis. In particular, exposure to effective
parenting techniques was a significant contributor to variations in self-control for the
hearing, but not the Deaf sample. Additionally, self-control did not fully mediate the
relationship between child-rearing experiences and norm violating behaviors for the
Deaf sample. Implications of these findings are discussed.
Keywords
self-control, cultural invariance, Deaf culture, parental management
Introduction
A General Theory of Crime has been tested numerous times, employing a wide array
of methodologies that have found empirical evidence providing support for the theory
(see de Ridder et al., 2012; Pratt & Cullen, 2000; Vazsonyi et al., 2017). Self-control
theory, as argued by Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990, pp. 174–179), is not conditioned
1Rochester Institute of Technology, NY, USA
Corresponding Author:
Tony R. Smith, Department of Criminal Justice, Rochester Institute of Technology, 93 Lomb Memorial
Drive, Rochester, NY 14623-5603, USA.
Email: trsgcj@rit.edu
1049186IJOXXX10.1177/0306624X211049186International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative CriminologySmith et al.
research-article2021
Smith et al. 525
by differences found across cultures. This cultural invariance hypothesis has previ-
ously been tested with a wide variety of cultural groups. These empirical studies, how-
ever, have not examined self-control theory among cultures that transcend race and
ethnicity and that are not bound by geography. In particular, self-control theory has not
been tested among the distinct culture established by the Deaf and hard of hearing
population. This current study attempts to add to the cross-cultural evaluations of A
General Theory of Crime drawing on samples from a mid-sized private university that
also houses an institute for Deaf and hard-of-hearing students.
Deaf Culture
The Deaf community represents a unique avenue to examine the cultural invariance of
self-control theory. According to Ladd (2003), the earliest use of the term “culture” to
describe the collective life of individuals with profound auditory loss began in the
1950s with the sociolinguist’s use of “subcultures” (Lunde, 1960), the “social and
cultural characteristics of deaf people” (Stokoe et al., 1965), and “Deaf community
life” (Higgins, 1980). One of the earliest definitions of Deaf culture was produced by
Padden (1980) when she referenced a “set of learned behaviors of a group of people
who have their own language, values, rules for behavior, and traditions” (p. 92). While
these early references to Deaf culture occasionally varied in their specific emphasis,
they shared a common understanding based on the distinction between the audiologi-
cal condition of not hearing, often denoted with a lowercase deaf, from the group
membership of individuals sharing a common language, institutions, norms, values,
and customs, and designated with the uppercase Deaf (Padden & Humphries, 1988).
In other words, “those within the deaf community who are culturally Deaf consider
themselves to be a linguistic and cultural minority, not people with a disability (Pray
& Jordan, 2010, p. 173).” According to Reagan (1995), “The Deaf cultural community
is, from the perspective of the sociocultural model of deafness, characterized by the
same kinds of elements that characterize any other cultural community, including: a
common, shared language (ASL), a shared awareness of Deaf cultural identity, dis-
tinctive behavioral norms and patterns, cultural artifacts, endogamous marital pat-
terns, a shared historical knowledge and awareness, and a network of voluntary,
in-group social organizations (Reagan, 1995, p. 243).” It has been estimated that
approximately half a million people in the United States view deafness from this cul-
tural framework (Lane, 2005). This figure, of course, does not include members of the
mainstream culture—for example, hearing friends and family, Deaf allies, ASL inter-
preters, et cetera—who may also subscribe to this cultural framework as well. Below
we provide a brief overview of the literature defining the Deaf culture that we offer in
support of the inclusion of this group in our cross-cultural test of self-control theory.
Some have argued that the most prominent feature or defining characteristic of
Deaf culture is language (Pray & Jordan, 2010). American Sign Language (ASL) is the
primary language used by Deaf native signers and it is estimated that ASL is the pre-
dominant language used by between 100,000 and 500,000 Americans (Padden, 1987).
Holcomb (2013) describes language as “[enabling] people in the community to have

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT