Do Defendants Pay What Juries Award? Post‐Verdict Haircuts in Texas Medical Malpractice Cases, 1988–2003

Published date01 March 2007
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/j.1740-1461.2007.00081.x
AuthorBernard Black,Charles Silver,David A. Hyman,Kathryn Zeiler,William M. Sage
Date01 March 2007
Do Defendants Pay What Juries
Award? Post-Verdict Haircuts in Texas
Medical Malpractice Cases, 1988–2003
David A. Hyman, Bernard Black, Kathryn Zeiler, Charles Silver,
and William M. Sage*
Legal scholars, legislators, policy advocates, and the news media frequently
use jury verdicts to draw conclusions about the performance of the tort
system. However, actual payouts can differ greatly from verdicts. We report
evidence on post-verdict payouts from the most comprehensive longitudinal
study of matched jury verdicts and payouts. Using data on all insured
medical malpractice claims in Texas from 1988–2003 in which the plaintiff
received at least $25,000 (in 1988 dollars) following a jury trial, we find that
most jury awards received “haircuts.” Seventy-five percent of plaintiffs
received a payout less than the adjusted verdict (jury verdict plus prejudg-
ment and postjudgment interest), 20 percent received the adjusted verdict
*Address correspondence to David A. Hyman, phone: 217-333-0061; email: dhyman@
law.uiuc.edu. Hyman is Professor of Law and Medicine and Galowich-Huizenga Faculty Scholar,
University of Illinois. Black is Hayden W. Head Regents Chair for Faculty Excellence, University
of Texas Law School, and Professor of Finance, University of Texas, Red McCombs School of
Business. Zeiler is Visiting Professor of Law, New York University School of Law, and Associate
Professor of Law, Georgetown University Law Center. Silver is McDonald Endowed Chair in
Civil Procedure, University of Texas Law School. Sage is James R. Dougherty Chair for Faculty
Excellence in Law, University of Texas School of Law, and Vice-Provost for Health Affairs,
University of Texas at Austin.
We owe special thanks to Fang Zhang and Myungho Paik for their work in analyzing the data,
and to Vicky Knox, Ken McDaniel, Clare Pramuk, and Brian Ryder at the Texas Department of
Insurance for patiently answering our many questions. For comments and suggestions, we thank
an anonymous referee, Jennifer Arlen, Ronen Avraham, Tom Baker, Ted Frank, Cathy Sharkey,
Martin Wells, participants at the RAND/JELS conference on medical malpractice (2006) and
the First Annual Conference on Empirical Legal Studies (University of Texas Law School, 2006),
and workshop and seminar participants at Berkeley, Georgetown, NYU, Northwestern, and
Vanderbilt University Schools of Law, and the Institute for Government and Public Affairs at the
University of Illinois. Funding for this study was provided by the Center on Lawyers, Civil Justice,
and the Media at the University of Texas School of Law, a research grant from the University of
Texas at Austin, and the Jon David and Elizabeth Epstein Program in Health Law and Policy at
the University of Illinois College of Law.
Journal of Empirical Legal Studies
Volume 4, Issue 1, 3–68, March 2007
©2007, Copyright the Authors
Journal compilation ©2007, Cornell Law School and Blackwell Publisher, Inc.
3
(within 2 percent), and 5 percent received more than the adjusted
verdict. Overall, plaintiffs received a mean (median) per-case haircut of 29
percent (19 percent), and an aggregate haircut of 56 percent, relative to the
adjusted verdict. The larger the verdict, the more likely and larger the
haircut. For cases with a positive adjusted verdict under $100,000, 47
percent of plaintiffs received a haircut, with a mean (median) per-case
haircut of 8 percent (2 percent). For cases with an adjusted verdict larger
than $2.5 million, 98 percent of plaintiffs received a haircut with a mean
(median) per-case haircut of 56 percent (61 percent). Insurance policy
limits are the most important factor in explaining haircuts. Caps on
damages in death cases and caps on punitive damages are also important,
but defendants often paid substantially less than the adjusted allowed
verdict. Remittitur accounts for a small percentage of the haircuts. Punitive
damage awards have only a small effect on payouts. Out-of-pocket payments
by physicians are rare, never large, and usually unrelated to punitive
damage awards. Most cases settle, presumably in the shadow of the outcome
if the case were to be tried. That outcome is not the jury award, but the
actual post-verdict payout. Because defendants rarely pay what juries award,
jury verdicts alone do not provide a sufficient basis for claims about the
performance of the tort system.
I. Introduction
Juries and jury verdicts occupy center stage in the political debate over tort
reform and in academic analyses of the tort system. In the political arena,
critics claim that juries are out of control and out of their depth, periodically
dispensing unjustified blockbuster verdicts, especially against defendants with
deep pockets. These critics argue that this “lawsuit lottery” encourages defen-
dants to settle even nonmeritorious cases, and imposes a sizeable “tort tax” on
the economy. Conversely, defenders argue that juries generally “get it right,”
and that blockbuster verdicts are rare and often reduced by judicial oversight.
Both sides support or oppose reforms based on their differing views of how
juries behave. In like fashion, legal scholars assume that most cases are
resolved in the shadow of what a jury would award, and an extensive literature
models litigation and settlement decisions in the shadow of trial outcomes.
Thus, jury verdicts are used as the principal endpoint in both the
political debate over tort reform and in academic analyses of litigation and
settlement dynamics. As one set of scholars explained, “jury trial verdicts
form the basis of what we think we know about tort litigation.”1
1Ostrom et al. (1992–1994:97, 98).
4Hyman et al.
However, using jury verdicts as the relevant endpoint can be mislead-
ing if post-verdict payouts differ significantly from jury awards. Downward
departures can result from judicial oversight (remittitur, judgment notwith-
standing the verdict (jnov), and appellate reversal), statutory damages caps,
and settlement dynamics (which are influenced by limits on collectibility).
Upward departures can result from prejudgment and postjudgment interest,
and settlement dynamics. Whatever the sources of these adjustments, poli-
cymakers should factor them into their assessments of the performance of
the tort system, and academics should consider them in analyzing litigation
and settlement behavior. In particular, we would expect cases to settle in the
shadow of what the plaintiff can expect to collect if the case is tried—not
solely in the shadow of the expected jury award.
Past studies of post-verdict adjustments and payouts are limited. Most
focus on judicial review of the verdict, and provide limited information on
actual payments by defendants and their insurers. Most past studies also
cover a limited time period and use data that are subject to sample selection
bias, typically hand-gathered from jury verdict reporters, court dockets, and
surveys.2
We employ a unique data set of all closed insured Texas medical
malpractice claims from 1988–2003 with payout over $25,000 to study jury
verdicts and the frequency, size, and reasons for differences between jury
awards and payouts.3We find that post-verdict payouts fall substantially short
of both “adjusted verdicts” (jury awards plus prejudgment and postjudgment
interest), and “adjusted allowed verdicts” (adjusted verdicts less the effects of
damage caps and remittitur). Stated differently, although jury verdicts and
payouts are correlated, most jury verdicts receive a substantial “haircut” when
they are paid by the defendant. In particular, we find that (amounts in 1988
dollars):4
Of 306 jury trials with plaintiff verdicts, 228 cases (75 percent) had
payment less than the adjusted verdict, 62 cases (20 percent) had
2See Section II for a summary of previous studies.
3This article is one of a series based on the Texas database. Other papers include Black et al.
(2005); Zeiler et al. (forthcoming).
4Unless otherwise indicated, all dollar amounts are in 1988 dollars; computed using the Con-
sumer Price Index for All Urban Consumers (annual average) as a price index. Source:
www.bls.gov/cpi/. To convert to 2006 dollars, multiply by 1.71.
Do Defendants Pay What Juries Award? 5

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT