Damage Anchors on Real Juries

AuthorBeth Murphy,Mary R. Rose,John Meixner,Shari Seidman Diamond
Published date01 December 2011
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/j.1740-1461.2011.01232.x
Date01 December 2011
Damage Anchors on Real Juriesjels_1232148..178
Shari Seidman Diamond, Mary R. Rose, Beth Murphy, and John Meixner*
Experiments reveal anchoring as a powerful force, even when participants see the anchor as
irrelevant. Here, we examine the reactions of real deliberating jurors to attorney damage
requests and concessions in 31 cases involving 33 plaintiffs in which the jury awarded
damages. Jurors were critical consumers of attorney suggestions. They reacted more nega-
tively to, and were less inf‌luenced by, plaintiff ad damnums for pain and suffering than to
damage requests in categories grounded in more objective evidence. Deliberations revealed
that jurors often perceive plaintiff ad damnums not only as irrelevant, but also as outrageous,
impressions ref‌lected in their verdicts. These f‌indings suggest that extreme plaintiff ad
damnums, including those without grounding in quantitative evidence from trial, may not
exert substantial undue inf‌luence.
I. Introduction
When the jury in an ordinary civil trial f‌inds the defendant liable, the jury’s next task is to
determine the amount of damage caused by the defendant’s acts. Most commentators agree
that this is a diff‌icult assignment,1and jurors as well seem to recognize that they have a
challenging task.2In part, the challenge arises from the minimal guidance the law provides
*Address correspondence to Shari S. Diamond, Northwestern University Law School, 357 E. Chicago Ave., Chicago,
IL 60611; email: s-diamond@law.northwestern.edu. Diamond is the Howard J. Trienens Professor of Law and
Professor of Psychology, Northwestern University and Research Professor, American Bar Foundation; Rose is Asso-
ciate Professor of Sociology and Law at the University of Texas at Austin; Murphy is Research Social Scientist,
American Bar Foundation; Meixner is a JD/Ph.D. student at Northwestern University.
This research was supported by research grants from the State Justice Institute (Grant SJI-97-N-247), the National
Science Foundation (Grant SBR9818806), and the American Bar Foundation, with additional support from North-
western University Law School and Duke University Law School. We are indebted to an enlightened group of Arizona
judges who, with an eye toward optimizing the jury trial, permitted the videotaping project to occur. For helpful
comments and suggestions on this article, we thank Valerie Hans and the participants at the Cornell Conference on
Judgment by Numbers, October 2010.
1See, e.g., Edie Greene & Brian H. Bornstein, Precious Little Guidance: Jury Instruction on Damage Awards, 6
Psychol. Pub. Pol’y & L. 743 (2000); Michael J. Saks, Lisa A. Hollinger, Roselle L. Wissler, David Lee Evans & Allen
J. Hart, Reducing Variability in Civil Jury Awards, 21 Law & Hum. Behav. 243 (1997); Roselle L. Wissler, Patricia F.
Kuehn & Michael J. Saks, Instructing Jurors on General Damages in Personal Injury Cases: Problems and Possibilities,
6 Psychol. Pub. Pol’y. & L. 712 (2000).
2See generally Shari Seidman Diamond, What Jurors Think: Expectations and Reactions of Citizens Who Serve as
Jurors, in Verdict: Assessing the Civil Jury(R. Litan ed., 1993).
Journal of Empirical Legal Studies
Volume 8, Issue S1, 148–178, December 2011
148
in the determination of damages.3In part, the diff‌iculty stems from the inherent uncer-
tainty of the projections that jurors are asked to make concerning, for example, likely future
medical expenses. In part, ambiguity arises as jurors must try to assess the value of the more
intangible losses associated with pain and suffering that have no ascertainable market
value.4
Faced with the diff‌icult task of determining damages by assessing the injury done to
the plaintiff and then translating the injury into an amount that will reasonably compensate
the plaintiff, jurors look for appropriate cues. A similar search occurs in many judgment
situations, and there is good evidence that decisionmakers commonly employ the cognitive
heuristic of anchoring and adjustment to assist them in simplifying their task.5That is, they
often identify an anchor that provides a starting point and, ultimately, although subject to
adjustment, the anchor inf‌luences their judgment.
In legal settings, jurors generally have access to a potentially potent anchor: attorney
damage recommendations. Attorneys in most jurisdictions are permitted to recommend
damage awards, with few limitations imposed on the way an attorney arrives at or presents
those suggested amounts.6Yet although most jurisdictions permit these attorney sugges-
tions, that permission is not without controversy, and the practice is forbidden in several
states. Here, for the f‌irst time, we are able to examine the role these potential anchors from
the attorneys play during the deliberations of real civil juries. We f‌ind evidence that jurors
are eager for input from the attorneys as they search for guideposts in determining awards
to appropriately compensate injured parties, but they are also critical consumers of that
attorney advice.
We begin Section II with a discussion of the task of determining damages and the
arguments for and against permitting attorneys to offer damage recommendations at trial.
The tension is between the benef‌its of providing useful guidance and the risk of distorting
jury judgments with undue inf‌luence. In Section III we examine research on anchoring
inside and outside the legal domain, a body of empirical research that suggests the potential
power of attorney damage suggestions. In Section IV, we outline our hypotheses about how
3Edie Greene & Brian H. Bornstein, Determining Damages: The Psychology of Jury Awards 20 (2003).
4See, e.g., Oscar G. Chase, Helping Jurors Determine Pain and Suffering Awards, 23 Hofstra L. Rev. 763, 765 (1995)
(“[pain and suffering] requires the monetization of a ‘product’ for which there is no market and therefore no market
price”).
5See, e.g., Thomas Mussweiler, The Malleability of Anchoring Effects, 49 Experimental Psychol. 67 (2002) (citing
results from a variety of domains: general knowledge, probability estimates, legal judgment, pricing decisions, and
negotiation).
6The one general exception is the generally forbidden so-called golden rule in which jurors are asked to put
themselves in the injured person’s place and render the verdict they would want to receive if they were in that person’s
position. See, e.g., Edwards v. City of Phila., 860 F.2d 568, 574 (3d Cir. 1988); Spray-Rite Serv. Corp. v. Monsanto Co.,
684 F.2d 1226, 1246 (7th Cir. 1982), aff’d, 465 U.S. 752 (1984); Loose v. Offshore Navigation, Inc., 670 F.2d 493, 496
(5th Cir. 1982). The argument against the use of the “golden rule” is that jurors are supposed to be impartial and
determine fair compensation. It is thus improper to attempt to draw them into a direct relationship with the plaintiff,
encouraging sympathy or bias. L.R. James, Annotation, Instructions in a Personal Injury Action Which, in Effect, Tell
Jurors that in Assessing Damages They Should Put Themselves in Injured Person’s Place, 96 A.L.R.2d 760 (2008).
Damage Anchors on Real Juries 149

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT