Criminal Sentencing by Preferred Numbers

Published date01 March 2020
AuthorMandeep K. Dhami,Sheila M. Bird,Andrew McGrath,Elizabeth Merrall,Ian K. Belton
Date01 March 2020
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/jels.12246
Journal of Empirical Legal Studies
Volume 17, Issue 1, 139–163, Mach 2020
Criminal Sentencing by Preferred Numbers
Mandeep K. Dhami,*Ian K. Belton, Elizabeth Merrall, Andrew McGrath,
and Sheila M. Bird
Criminal sentencing is a complex cognitive activity often performed by the unaided mind
under suboptimal conditions. As such, sentencers may not behave according to policy,
guidelines, or training. We analyzed the distribution of sentences meted out in one year in
two different jurisdictions (i.e., England and Wales, and New South Wales, Australia). We
reveal that sentencers prefer certain numbers when meting out sentence lengths
(in custody and community service) and amounts (for fines/compensation). These “com-
mon doses” accounted for over 90 percent of sentences in each jurisdiction. The size of
these doses increased as sentences became more severe, and doses followed a logarithmic
pattern. Our findings are compatible with psychological research on preferred numbers
and are reminiscent of Weber’s and Fechner’s laws. The findings run contrary to argu-
ments against efforts to reduce judicial discretion, and potentially undermine the notion
of individualized justice, as well as raise questions about the (cost) effectiveness of
sentencing.
I. Introduction
Criminal sentencing represents a key stage of the criminal justice process where the
courts dispose of offenders who have pled guilty to an offense or been convicted of one.
The punishments meted out to offenders may give them their just deserts, incapacitate
or deter them from committing crimes in the future, rehabilitate them, or enable them
to make reparations. Consequently, sentencing is often predicated on the principle that
each case is unique and so dealt with on its own merits. This notion of “individualized jus-
tice” is fundamental to the sentencing framework of many jurisdictions. For instance, the
U.S. Sentencing Commission (2018:14) holds to the belief that sentences are based on
the “individualized facts of the case.” The Sentencing Council for England and Wales
*Address correspondence to Professor Mandeep K. Dhami, Department of Psychology, Middlesex University, The
Burroughs, Hendon, London, UK, NW4 4BT; email: m.dhami@mdx.ac.uk. Belton is at University of Strathclyde,
UK; Merrall is with S-cubed ApS, Denmark; McGrath is at Charles Sturt University, Australia; Bird is at University
of Cambridge, UK.
Thank you to the Sentencing Council for England and Wales, U.K. Ministry of Justice, and the New South
Wales Bureau of Crime Statistics and Research for providing access to the data sources. We also thank John Irwin
for his feedback on a draft of the article. The dataset for England and Wales (but not all of the analyses) presented
here formed part of the third author’s PhD thesis (Merrall, 2012).
139
declares that “[i]t is important to note that every criminal case is unique” (2015:9,
emphasis in original). Finally, the New South Wales, Australia sentencing bench book
(Judicial Commission of New South Wales 2014) quotes the court in R v. Engert as stating
that “[i]n every case, what is called for is the making of a discretionary decision in the
light of the circumstances of the individual case” (1995:2010).
Indeed, judicial discretion is considered essential for producing individualized jus-
tice, and jurisdictions differ in how much discretion is afforded to sentencers. In some
jurisdictions, such as in the United States, sentencers are expected to follow highly struc-
tured, numerical, grid-based guidelines that specify the factors that should determine a
sentence (e.g., offense seriousness and criminal history) and constrain the sentences that
are meted out (see Dhami et al. 2015). Other jurisdictions, such as England and Wales,
have opted for more flexible, text-based guidance that leave room for the exercise of dis-
cretion in an apparent effort to enable individualized justice (Gage 2008). Yet, in other
jurisdictions, such as in Australia, sentencers have eschewed guidelines altogether
(beyond guideline judgments) because these do not sufficiently prioritize individualiza-
tion (Australian Law Reform Commission 2006).
The exercise of discretion may, however, be problematic. Sentencers must search
for, and attend to, relevant information (e.g., harm caused, culpability of the offender),
weight it appropriately, and integrate it in order to determine a sentence. As such, sen-
tencing is a cognitive activity that may be affected by the attentional, memory, and
processing limitations of the human mind. Indeed, research has found that sentencers
may ignore or take insufficient account of legal factors (Konecˇni & Ebbesen 1982; von
Helversen & Rieskamp 2009) and may be unduly influenced by extra-legal factors
(e.g., Daly & Bordt 1995; Mitchell 2005; Steffensmeier et al. 1998). Sentencers may also
be biased by irrelevant demands or anchors (Englich et al. 2006; Guthrie et al. 2007), the
order in which cases are presented, and even the gradation of the custodial sentencing
scale used (Rachlinski et al. 2015).
In the present research, we explore the idea that sentencing decisions may be
unduly influenced by the sentencing options available to the sentencer. Sentencing
options are typically measured on continuous scales, namely, length of time (e.g., custody,
community sentence) and amount of money (i.e., fine, compensation). There is a growing
body of psychological literature on how people use continuous scales, and we consider the
implications of this for criminal punishment.
II. Preferred Numbers, Just Noticeable Differences and
Logarithmic Scaling
Psychological research has established that people use certain numbers much more fre-
quently than others when they are estimating the level of a stimulus (Baird et al. 1970;
Baird & Noma 1975; Stevens 1975). For example, when asked to generate numbers
between 1 and 100, multiples of five and 10 are more commonly selected than other
numbers (Baird & Noma 1975; Plug 1977). Multiples of five and 10 also occur more
140 Dhami et al.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT