Coercion and Provocation

DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1177/0022002720957078
Published date01 February 2021
Date01 February 2021
Subject MatterArticles
Article
Coercion
and Provocation
Allan Dafoe
1,2
, Sophia Hatz
3
,
and Baobao Zhang
1,4
Abstract
Threats and force, by increasing expected costs, should reduce the target’s resolve.
However, they often seem to increase resolve. We label this phenomenon provo-
cation. We review instances of apparent provocation in interstate relations and offer
a theory based on the logic of reputation and honor. We also consider alternative
explanations: confounding or mis-imputation of resolve; revelation of information,
character, or capabilities; or generalized sunk cost reasoning. Using survey experi-
ments, we systematically evaluate whether provocation exists and what may account
for it. We employ design-based causal inference techniques—a hypothetical natural
experiment, a placebo treatment, and ruling out mediators—to evaluate our key
hypotheses. We find strong evidence of provocation and suggestive evidence that it
arises from considerations of honor, vengeance, and reputation. Our experimental
design minimizes the risk that this result arises from our alternative explanations.
Keywords
interstate conflict, international security, use of force, militarized interstate disputes
Overview
A fundamental assumption in our understanding of conflict is that threats and force
function as a means of coercion. The logic is simple: the use of threatened or actual
1
Future of Humanity Institute, University of Oxford, United Kingdom
2
Department of Politics and International Relations, University of Oxford, United Kingdom
3
Department of Peace and Conflict Research, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
4
Department of Government, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA
Corresponding Author:
Baobao Zhang, Cornell University, 214 White Hall, Ithaca, New York 14853, USA.
Email: baobaozhangresearch@gmail.com
Journal of Conflict Resolution
2021, Vol. 65(2-3) 372-402
ªThe Author(s) 2020
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/0022002720957078
journals.sagepub.com/home/jcr
harm increases a target’s actual and expected costs of resistance, reducing the
target’s resolve, or determination to resist. And yet, contrary to this straightforward
logic, threats and violence often seem to result in provocation; they appear to
increase the resolve of a target, in some cases leading to greater resistance and
retaliation. The September 11 at tacks were arguably an attempt by al- Qaeda to
persuade the US to reduce its military presence in Saudi Arabia and support for
Israel. And yet, far from coercing the American public against involvement in the
Middle East, the attacks of 9/11 provoked the US into invading two countries in the
region, as well as maintaining its previous commitments. Historians and scholars
have documented many such examples throughout history and across cultures, in
interstate and intrastate conflicts (Carlisle 2007; Kagan 1995; Kalyvas 2006; O’Neill
1999). In many tense areas of the world—such as the South and East China Seas, the
Balkans, Korea, and India/Pakistan—analysts express concern that even a minor
“provocation,” such as the collision of military planes or an exchange of fire across
borders, could spiral out of control into a war.
Given our assumptions about the coercive power of actual and threatened harm,
how can we explain its apparently ubiquitous provocation effect? This paper
addresses the puzzle of coercion and provocation. The phenomenon that threats and
violence can provoke resistance and retaliation is recognized (Lichbach 1987;
Myerson 2007), and theories describe why actors might use threats and violence
to provoke (Carter 2016; Kydd and Walter 2006; De Mesquita and Dickson 2007;
Rocco and Ballo 2008). Nevertheless, we lack an explanation for provocation as a
reaction in a target. Studying the phenomenon of provocation is also complicated by
observational challenges: increased resolve is unobservable, inferring resolve from
actions is problematic, and historical counterfactuals are elusive. In this paper, we
offer a theory for how and why provocation occurs based on a review of historical
examples, inductive theory-building, and scenario-based survey experiments. We
employ principles of design-based causal inference in the design and analysis of our
experiments in order to evaluate the processes by which a minor “provocation” (the
aggressive maneuvering of military planes) translates into public support for
escalation.
We first review salient historical examples of apparent provocation: cases where
the application of harm in a coercive setting—such as a threat, aggressive maneu-
vering, or an attack—appeared to substantially increase the target’s resolve or desire
for retaliation. From this review we identify an explanation for provocation that we
focus on in this paper: concern for reputation and honor (E
1
). E
1
emerges from the
observation that threats, aggression, and violence are often perceived as tests of
resolve and matters of national honor. Perceived in this way, coercive acts often
put reputation (for resolve) and honor at stake. By resisting coercion, targets signal
to others that they will not allow themselves to be coerced and that coercion is not
effective against them. This reputational logic could emerge from many different
processes. It could emerge from rational calculation of the value of a deterrent
reputation (Sartori 2002; Weisiger and Yarhi-Milo 2015, §10); from cultures that
Dafoe et al. 373

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT