An Evaluation of the Impact of Goal Setting and Cell Phone Calls on Juvenile Rearrests

Published date01 December 2016
AuthorStephen J. Bahr,Lance D. Erickson,David J. Cherrington
Date01 December 2016
DOI10.1177/0306624X15588549
Subject MatterArticles
International Journal of
Offender Therapy and
Comparative Criminology
2016, Vol. 60(16) 1816 –1835
© The Author(s) 2015
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0306624X15588549
ijo.sagepub.com
Article
An Evaluation of the Impact
of Goal Setting and Cell
Phone Calls on Juvenile
Rearrests
Stephen J. Bahr1, David J. Cherrington1,
and Lance D. Erickson1
Abstract
Using a sample of 256 juvenile offenders who were randomly assigned to treatment
or control groups, this study evaluates a cognitive-behavioral program that combines
cognitive training, goal setting, and a phone-coach follow-up. The training involved
six classroom sessions where participants received instruction and help in creating
individualized goals. After attending the classes, participants received automated
phone calls twice a day for up to a year. During the year following the program, the
treatment and control groups were not significantly different in whether or not they
were rearrested or in total rearrests. However, the total number of calls received
had a significant negative association with whether or not they were rearrested for a
felony and with the total number of felony rearrests.
Keywords
rearrests, felony, recidivism, goal setting, cognitive training, technology
Juvenile crime continues to be a serious social problem in the United States. From
1985 to 2011, delinquency caseloads involving drug offenses nearly doubled, and per-
son offenses increased by 72%. In 2011, juvenile courts in the United States processed
1.24 million cases and supervised more than 31 million youth (Hockenberry &
Puzzanchera, 2014). Although the number of cases handled by juvenile courts
decreased substantially from 2002 to 2011, a large number of juveniles continue to
commit serious offenses. In 2011, juveniles between the ages of 10 and 17 were 10.7%
1Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, USA
Corresponding Author:
Stephen J. Bahr, Department of Sociology, Brigham Young University, 2031 JFSB, Provo, UT 84602, USA.
Email: stephen_bahr@byu.edu
588549IJOXXX10.1177/0306624X15588549International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative CriminologyBahr et al.
research-article2015
Bahr et al. 1817
of the U.S. population but accounted for 13% of all violent arrests and 20% of all
arrests for property crimes (Puzzanchera, 2013).
In response to these trends, many programs have been created to help rehabilitate
juvenile offenders. Although there is a growing body of research evaluating juvenile
programs, many programs have not received rigorous evaluation (Jewell, Malone,
Rose, Sturgeon, & Owens, 2015; Lipsey, 2014).
This report is part of an ongoing evaluation of a two-part rehabilitation program
called RealVictory that combines six cognitive training sessions and twice-a-day
phone calls for the following year. Previous evaluations of the program found that
treatment participants had fewer rearrests than comparable controls using a sample of
70 juvenile probationers (Burraston, Cherrington, & Bahr, 2012). The purpose of the
present study is to evaluate the effectiveness of the RealVictory program on reducing
rearrests among a sample of 256 juvenile offenders from three different treatment
settings.
Effective Juvenile Rehabilitation Programs
There have been a number of assessments of “what works” in existing crime preven-
tion, correctional, and reentry programs (Boyum, Caulkins, & Kleiman, 2011;
MacKenzie, 2006; Seiter & Kadela, 2003; Sherman, Farrington, Welsh, & MacKenzie,
2002; Wormwith et al., 2007). Although there has been a substantial amount of research
on treatment effectiveness, many programs are not adequately evaluated, and few
policy decisions in corrections use scientific evidence to assist in making informed
decisions (MacKenzie, 2006). In addition, evidence that a program works in one set-
ting is no guarantee that it will work on another group in a different setting (Lipsey,
2014). There is a critical need for research that evaluates the effectiveness of different
juvenile programs and identifies the conditions under which different types of treat-
ments are effective (Boyum et al., 2011; Cullen & Jonson, 2011; Jewell et al., 2015;
Lipsey, 2014; Wormwith et al., 2007).
Reviews of existing research indicate that a number of different types of programs
are promising, particularly cognitive-behavioral therapy. In a meta-analysis of 400
studies treating juvenile delinquents, Lipsey (1995) observed that the average recidi-
vism rate was about 50% for the control group compared with 45% for the treatment
group, a 10% reduction in recidivism. Lipsey, Chapman, and Landenberger (2001)
examined 14 experiments and found that recidivism rates of those in treatment groups
were about two thirds as high as those in the control groups. The results appeared to be
somewhat stronger among juveniles than among adults. In a meta-analysis of 58 pro-
grams, Landenberger and Lipsey (2005) reported that, on average, treatment partici-
pants had a recidivism rate 25% lower than control participants. The programs were
more effective if they treated high-risk offenders, were implemented well, and included
cognitive-behavioral treatment methods.
In their review of various meta-analyses, Lipsey and Cullen (2007) concluded that
cognitive-behavioral programs tend to reduce recidivism although the results are
rather modest. Effective cognitive-behavioral programs teach prosocial attitudes and

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT